Enhancing Motivation to Think Critically

Journal: Journal of Higher Education Research DOI: 10.32629/jher.v3i4.1019

Xiaotong Chen

The University of Sydney, NSW, 2006, Australia

Abstract

In the recent few decades, critical thinking (CT) has been regarded as one of the main thinking skills utilised in academic fields and school learning. People usually need to think critically when they tend to solve a problem, assess an argument, decide about a belief, or make a decision (Valenzuela, Nieto, & Saiz, 2011). Critical thinking is a higher-order process, which requires self-determination, reflection, effort, self-control, and metacognition (Mertes, 1991, cited in Fahim & Hajimaghsoodi, 2014). CT also can help students organise their thinking structure, utilise supported standards and finally promote their thinking quality (Fahim & Hajimaghsoodi, 2014). This is why increasing numbers of universities and school establish critical thinking courses for students. Although critical thinking is a significant ability in academic learning, some students have not realise its importance. However, the element component skills of CT are deductive and inductive reasoning, problem-solving and decision making (Saiz & Rivas, 2008, cited in Valenzuela et al., 2011), which assist students when they are studying or doing researches. Thus, it is crucial to encourage students to think critically. For many students, although they have already required the skills of critical thinking, they are unwilling to use it in learning. Facione (2000) points out that some people may specialise in CT, but they are not interested in addressing an issue within critically way.

Keywords

critical thinking, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, cognitive process, cognitive load

References

[1] Facione, P. A. (2000). The disposition toward critical thinking: Its character, measurement, and relationship to critical thinking skill. Informal logic, 20(1).
[2] Fahim, M., & Hajimaghsoodi, A. (2014). The relationship between motivation and critical thinking ability of Iranian EFL learners. International Journal of Language Learning and Applied Linguistics World, 454.
[3] Krawczyk, D., Gazzaley, A., D&Amp;Apos, & Esposito, M. (2007). Reward modulation of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive working memory task. Brain Research, 1141(1), 168–177.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.052
[4] Kuldas, S., Satyen, L., Ismail, H. N., & Hashim, S. (2014). Greater cognitive effort for better learning: Tailoring an instructional design for learners with different levels of knowledge and motivation. Psychologica Belgica, 54, 350-373.
[5] McDevitt, T. M., & Ormrod, J. E. (2004). Child development: Educating and working with children and adolescents (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
[6] Miele, D., & Wigfield, A. (2014). Quantitative and Qualitative Relations Between Motivation and Critical-Analytic Thinking. Educational Psychology Review, 26(4), 519–541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9282-2

Copyright © 2022 Xiaotong Chen

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License